Translate this page:
Please select your language to translate the article


You can just close the window to don't translate
Library
Your profile

Back to contents

Psychology and Psychotechnics
Reference:

Neuropsychological syndromes of disorders of mnestic activity in patients with strokes in the right (subdominant) hemisphere

Selkin Mikhail Dmitrievich

Assistant to of Department of the General and Clinical Psychology of the First St. Petersburg State Medical University named of the Academician I.P. Pavlov

197022, Russia, Saint Petersburg, Lva Tolstogo str., 6-8

medpsiholog@bk.ru

DOI:

10.7256/2454-0722.2024.2.70524

EDN:

QTHJVL

Received:

18-04-2024


Published:

25-04-2024


Abstract: The work carried out a neuropsychological assessment of different types of memory and determined the profiles of mnestic disorders depending on the type of hemispheric asymmetry and localization of the lesion. The analysis of the features of the course of stroke in the right (subdominant) hemisphere was carried out. The main symptoms of impaired types of memory, for which the subdominant hemisphere is responsible, were studied. The presented data correlate with the data found in the world literature: stroke in the subdominant hemisphere has a less pronounced effect on memory impairment, is associated with a better prognosis, while visual memory, spatial, logical, and mechanical suffer more often. A detailed topographic analysis of the syndromatics is presented, comparing instrumental data on visualization of the lesion and psychological tests that reveal a deficiency of a particular type of memory. The data obtained make it possible to identify targets and opportunities for early neuropsychological correction of disorders by attracting compensatory brain capabilities, which is important when planning an individual rehabilitation program. The analysis of mnestic activity in patients with strokes in the right (subdominant) hemisphere was carried out. The applied set of patho- and neuropsychological methods: The Wexler memory scale, Indirect memorization according to A.N. Leontiev, the "Pictograms" technique, Benton's Visual retention, the "Memorizing 10 words" Technique, the "Memorizing faces" technique, the "Memorizing shapes" method., the MOSS test. For the first time, a comprehensive clinical, psychological and experimental psychological study was conducted, which made it possible to characterize the quality and hierarchy of impaired and preserved types of mental activity in the defeat of the right (subdominant) hemisphere. For the first time, a comparative analysis of disorders of the mnestic activity of the dominant and subdominant hemispheres of the brain was carried out, which allowed us to identify the specifics of memory disorders depending on the lateralization of lesions. The novelty is also represented by the systematization of mnestic disorders carried out by the author, accompanied by the isolation of neuropsychological syndromes of disorders of mnestic activity in the defeat of various parts of the right (subdominant) hemisphere in the acute period of syndromes of occipital, temporal, parietal lobe lesions and their combinations. Based on the data obtained, the necessity of studying the features of mnestic activity in order to restore and further rehabilitate higher mental functions in patients with cerebral infarction in the right hemisphere is justified.


Keywords:

Cognitive impairment, Neuropsychological diagnostics, Subdominant hemisphere, Brain infarction, Mnestic function, Hemispheric asymmetry, Neuropsychological syndromes, Neuropsychocorrection, Compensatory mechanisms, Disorders of cerebral circulation

This article is automatically translated.

Introduction

It is generally believed that ischemic lesion of the dominant hemisphere is a serious disorder that greatly affects the patient's condition and worsens the prognosis for rehabilitation, work capacity and quality of life. Thus, it is known that a violation in the basin of the left carotid artery leads to a pronounced cognitive deficit in 46% of cases [17].

At the same time, damage to the vascular basin supplying the subdominant hemisphere causes cognitive deficits in 15% of cases [18] and this is regardless of the severity of the lesion, that is, patients with both transient attacks and irreversible deeply disabling vascular events are included in the statistics [20]. One way or another, any suffered cerebrovascular accident is a serious case affecting the further fate of the patient. All this points to the enormous medical and social significance of the problem.

To date, medical practice has presented data indicating the proven effectiveness of neuropsychological rehabilitation techniques, the most effective is a technique that works according to the mechanism of intersystem functional compensation. Such therapy is a factor in improving prognosis, regardless of the patient's age, gender, initial cognitive status and vascular history. At the same time, the maximum effectiveness of neuropsychological and neurodynamic therapy can be achieved by purposefully training the lost function, in this case, a type of mnestic disorder. Despite the fact that determining the lesion in modern medicine is not difficult due to visualization methods, there is no clear understanding of the attachment of a particular function of higher nervous activity to a specific site. Of course, Brodman fields effectively map the cortex and are of great neurological and neurosurgical importance, but at the same time they cannot be a good assistant for a psychologist and psychotherapist.

Recently, many innovative papers have been published studying the function of the medial temporal lobe. There are works indicating that damage to the temporal lobe is associated with impaired memory for new facts, faces, events, places [3; 9]. These disorders are attributed to different areas of the medial temporal lobe, but the issue has not been fully studied, due to the complexity. In practice, difficulties arise with the selection of patients with such a local lesion, it is difficult to conduct their research (usually due to the severity of the general condition), it is also difficult to conduct research on laboratory animals due to excessively difficult access and the great danger of damaging "superfluous" structures by coagulating the studied area of the brain (that is, turning off the function, which he is responsible for).

Recent studies in rats, monkeys and in the clinic have shifted from studying the medial temporal lobe to the hippocampus and parahippocampal furrows. Finally, the most complete study of the issue was presented by the authors [13], who noticed that verbal memory is impaired in left-sided lesions, and spatial memory is impaired in right-sided lesions. It turned out that subjects with a right-sided lesion of the temporal lobe were worse at remembering the location of objects presented to them 3 minutes ago (while the patients retained spatial memory for the first minutes). The authors concluded that the subdominant hemisphere, namely, the subdominant temporal lobe, interacts extremely actively with the hippocampus in encoding spatial memory, moreover, participates in its storage, starting from a few minutes after memorization [1; 5; 9; 10]

This theory correlates perfectly with newer studies [2]. Great importance is given to the right parahippocampal gyrus, as well as the right hippocampus. At the same time, another recent study forms a new paradigm: a right-sided lesion cannot completely "turn off" the function of spatial thinking. Only a large part of its limitation occurs. That is, the function of spatial memory is mainly assumed by the subdominant hemisphere. At the same time, the structures of the dominant hemisphere also play an important role. Maguire et al. It has been shown that when solving difficult spatial problems, mapping, and route planning, right-sided parahippocampal structures work cooperatively with left-sided ones.

It should be noted that visual memory, functioning through the work of the temporal lobe, plays an extremely important social role. In isolation, this lesion is extremely rare [4]. However, laboratory studies allow us to delve into the issue of visual memory impairment and the mechanisms of its compensation after any damage to the cortex [14; 15].

Friendly interhemispheric interaction forms an adequate activity, which can be judged by the quality of memory. When such a synergy is violated, the unaffected area is disinhibited due to the disappearance that balances it [8; 9].

Experiments were conducted on the separate and friendly involvement of the hemispheres in the assessment of visual information and memorization. It turned out that if visual information enters only the subdominant hemisphere (and the dominant one is in short supply, that is, visual information does not enter it from the corresponding field of vision), there is a disinhibition of the subdominant hemisphere, which leads to its overload and the appearance of more errors in recall. The situation worsens when the caused deficiency in the activity of the dominant hemisphere is supplemented by the need to carry out mathematical calculations of the number that needs to be remembered [11].

The above-described studies indicate that the disappearance of the functioning of a site in the subdominant hemisphere entails an asymmetric shift in activity towards the dominant hemisphere, which leads to disinhibition of functional areas and decreased productivity with additional cognitive load [6].

Deterioration of short-term memory is an important symptom of damage to both the dominant and subdominant hemispheres. This symptom is not specific, it has been described in several studies. The peculiarity of this symptom is reversibility.

Of course, it is rare to talk about the complete restoration of short-term memory, according to some data, even with intensive cognitive training, it is possible to achieve the initial quality of memory in 40 percent of cases.

The diagnosis of this symptom is reduced to a simple test for memorizing 10 words according to the method of A.R. Luria [12]. A satisfactory result is the reproduction of 7 ± 2 words, if it is not known how many points the patient scored before ONMC, if it is known how much the patient memorized before ONMC, then this figure is the minimum goal of rehabilitation.

Correction of this symptom is important for the complete rehabilitation of the patient. An extremely effective method of correction is a combination of cognitive gymnastics with physical exercises. The combination of general physical exercises adapted to the patient's condition with game exercises like "repeat after me" or "repeat the movements that I will name" [18] accelerates both spontaneous memory recovery by the mechanism of disinhibition and improves memory due to the formation of cross-functional communications of the central nervous system.

Assessment of the affected area and the "dropped out" mnestic function can occur during cognitive testing, as well as in the assessment of symptoms and syndromes.

It is the assessment of the symptoms and syndromes (that is, the clinic) of the lesion that makes it possible to plan further cognitive therapy. In this regard, the topic of the work is extremely relevant - a review of the described syndrome in patients with damage to the subdominant hemisphere.

The purpose of this study was to analyze the structure of clinical symptoms and features of impaired mnestic function in patients with acute cerebral catastrophe in the right (subdominant) hemisphere.

Research methods and principles

An analysis of the clinical manifestations of right-sided ONMC in patients of the neurological department with a dominant left hemisphere was carried out. A group of patients with right-sided ONMC, consisting of 36 respondents, was selected. The same number of people were in the comparison group.

The group includes patients without severe disorders of consciousness, motor skills and cognitive functions. The observation was carried out for 14 days, which corresponds to the acute period of ONMC. 3 data measurements were performed: upon admission, after 7 days and upon discharge (14± 1 day).

The compared groups were comparable in terms of sex ratio, age 46± 0.9 years, level of education, percentage of people engaged in labor activity at the time of the incident.

A set of patho- and neuropsychological methods was used to assess memory: "Wexler Memory Scale"), Indirect memorization according to A.N. Leontiev, the "Pictograms" technique, Benton's Visual retention, the "Memorizing 10 words" Technique, the "Memorizing faces" Technique, the "Memorizing shapes" Technique, the MoSS test.

Main results

The results of the MoHS test were compared on 5 ± 2 days of hospital stay (Table 1).

 Table 1. MoHS test results

Groups of subjects

The average values are ± standard.deviation

(M ± ?)

The reliability of the differences, p

Group 1 (lesion in the dominant (left) hemisphere)

17±4,07

0,045

Group 2 (lesion in the subdominant (right) hemisphere)

22±2,95

The Montreal Cognitive Assessment Test is a simple screening method for assessing cognitive impairment (including memory). In the conducted study, the vast majority of patients had cognitive impairments, and their severity varied. The analysis showed significant differences between the two groups of subjects in terms of cognitive functions. Statistical analysis revealed that the confidence level (p<0.05) indicates statistically significant differences in the form of more pronounced cognitive impairment in patients of group 1 (with focal lesion in the dominant hemisphere). At the same time, patients of the 2nd group (with lesions in the subdominant hemisphere) showed less pronounced cognitive impairment according to the results of the same technique.

Further, more detailed testing was carried out using techniques for different types of memory. The results of testing patients in the acute period of ONMC are presented below (Table 2).

Table 2

Comparative analysis of mental activity in patients with lesions of the dominant and subdominant hemispheres at the time of admission to the hospital

Test

The sum of the ranks

U-criteria

Z

The level of significance of the differences, p

Patients with lesions in the right hemisphere (n=36)

Patients with lesions in the left hemisphere (n=36)

Stories

1410

1075

255

4,09

0,001

The numbers are in direct order

1477

1008

188

4,88

0,001

The numbers are in reverse order

1277

1209

389

2,5

0,01

Pair associations

1526

959

139

5,47

0,001

Indirectly. memorization

1313

1173

353

2,93

0,001

Pictograms

1285

1200

380

2,6

0,001

Memorizing faces

594

1891

129

-5,58

0,001

Memorizing shapes

685

1801

220

-4,51

0,001

The indicators of verbal-logical, operative, associative memory and indirect memorization significantly worsened in patients in both groups, however, patients with right-sided showed the best result, that is, ONMC did not have such a strong effect on memory deficit.

The analysis of the ONMC clinic in the right (subdominant) hemisphere, depending on the site of the lesion, was carried out (Table 3).

 

Table 3

Descriptive characteristics of mnestic activity in patients with subdominant (right) hemisphere lesion depending on the affected area

Indicators

The affected area

Average + standard. Deviation

Median

Min

Max

Stories

Dark.

9,15±4,47

8,0

1,5

15,0

Visoch.

7,4±4,3

8,3

0

14,5

The back of my head.

6,5±0,9

6,5

5,5

7,5

The numbers are in direct order

Dark.

5,2±0,9

5,5

4,0

6,0

Visoch.

5,6±1,3

6,0

4,0

8,0

The back of my head.

6,5±1,0

6,0

5,0

8,0

The numbers are in reverse order

Dark.

3,6±0,8

4,0

2,0

5,0

Visoch.

3,4±1,2

3,5

2,0

5,0

The back of my head.

4,1±1,1

4,5

2,0

5,0

Pair associations

Dark.

15,2±5,3

18,3

7,5

20,0

Visoch.

13,2±4,2

11,7

9,0

20,0

The back of my head.

10,1±0,8

10,0

9,0

11,5

Indirectly. Memorization

Dark.

67,7±26,2

72,7

13,0

100,0

Visoch.

73,7±33,2

90,0

14,2

100,0

The back of my head.

63,4±19,3

68,1

17,8

78,5

Pictograms

Dark.

57,0±25,5

67,7

0

80,7

Visoch.

53,4±23,4

51,6

0

80,7

The back of my head.

55,2±6,7

53,3

46,7

66,7

Visual retention

Dark.

5,2±1,7

5,5

3,0

8,0

Visoch.

3,7±2,7

3,0

0

8,0

The back of my head.

2,6±1,3

3,0

0

4,0

10 words (1)

Dark.

4,6±1,4

5,0

3,0

6,0

Visoch.

3,9±1,6

4,0

1,0

6,0

The back of my head.

4,5±0,5

4,5

4,0

5,0

10 words (2)

Dark.

5,7±1,3

6,0

4,0

8,0

Visoch.

5,5±2,0

6,5

1,0

7,0

The back of my head.

5,3±0,5

5,0

5,0

6,0

10 words (3)

Dark.

6,7±1,5

7,0

3,0

8,0

Visoch.

6,1±2,4

7,0

1,0

8,0

The back of my head.

5,8±0,8

6,0

5,0

7,0

10 words (4)

Dark.

6,7±1,6

7,0

3,0

9,0

Visoch.

6,7±2,7

8,0

1,0

10,0

The back of my head.

6,3±0,8

6,0

5,0

8,0

10 words (5)

Dark.

6,9±2,0

7,0

2,0

9,0

Visoch.

6,8±2,6

8,0

2,0

9,0

The back of my head.

6,2±0,9

6,5

5,0

7,0

10 words (retention)

Dark.

4,5±2,5

4,0

0

8,0

Visoch.

3,0±1,3

3,0

0

5,0

The back of my head.

4,1±2,9

5,0

0

7,0

Memorizing faces

Dark.

3,4±0,7

3,5

2,0

4,0

Visoch.

4,9±1,7

5,5

2,0

7,0

The back of my head.

3,0±0,8

3,0

2,0

4,0

Memorizing shapes

Dark.

3,1±1,3

3,0

1,0

5,0

Visoch.

6,4±3,5

8,0

1,0

9,0

The back of my head.

2,1±0,6

2,0

1,0

3,0

In a group of 10 patients with lesions in the occipital lobe of the right hemisphere, a reduced ability to form semantic connections was revealed. According to the method of "Story Reproduction", the average value was 6.5 ± 0.9 points out of a maximum of 23, which corresponds to 30% of the norm (80-100%). Such a low indicator indicates significant difficulties in perceiving and understanding a coherent text.

Along with this, this study notes the possible involvement of the temporal lobe of the right hemisphere in the pathological process. The difference in indicators between the occipital and temporal lobes is insignificant: in the study of visual-verbal memory using the "Paired Associations" method, the average value was 10.1 ± 0.8 points out of 21, which corresponds to 52% of the norm (80-100%). This result indicates difficulties in establishing associative links between visual and verbal stimuli.

Damage to the occipital lobe of the right hemisphere is also accompanied by a decrease in visual memory. The "Visual retention" technique revealed an average value of 2.6 ± 1.3 points out of 10, which is lower than the standard indicators (7-10 points). This disorder manifests itself in difficulties in memorizing and reproducing visual images.

The study by E.D. Chomskaya showed that damage to the left hemisphere in the occipital lobe also leads to visual memory disorders, especially in relation to visual stimuli. At the same time, the volume of visual-speech memory narrows, which makes it difficult to memorize and reproduce coherent visual-verbal material.

The combined lesion of the parietal and occipital lobes of the right hemisphere causes pronounced disorders of indirect memorization, memory for faces and geometric shapes. Patients have difficulty using auxiliary tools (for example, prepared maps) to improve the efficiency of memorization. The technique of "Indirect memorization according to A.N. Leontiev" showed average values of 63.4 ± 19.3 and 67.7 ± 26.2 points out of 90-100.

When the parietal lobe of the left hemisphere is affected with the involvement of the occipital regions, memory disorders for visual images are noted. The technique of "Memorizing shapes" revealed average values of 2.1 ± 0.6 and 3.1 ± 1.3 points out of 7-10, which indicates difficulties in memorizing and reproducing geometric shapes.

In general, damage to the occipital-temporal-parietal parts of the brain has a complex effect on various aspects of memory. Violations of visual, associative and episodic memory lead to a decrease in the ability to perceive, understand and remember information, which significantly complicates the adaptation of patients to the environment and daily activities.

With insufficient functioning of the temporal lobe of the right hemisphere (PV), violations of operational (fixation) memory are observed, which manifests itself in low indicators in the test "Memorizing 10 words" at the first presentation: 3.9 ± 1.6 (with a norm of 7 ± 2 words). This may be caused by the spontaneity of cognitive activity, general asthenization and an insufficient level of spontaneous initiation of activity. Similar disorders can be observed when the same area is affected in the left hemisphere. However, the process of further memorization of information indicates the involvement of the parietal and occipital lobes in it, which contributes to the effective retention of information.

The temporal lobe plays an important role in long-term memory and recall of auditory-speech information. A low level of logical memorization by visual reinforcement using drawings using the "Pictogram" technique is associated with insufficient functioning of the temporal and occipital lobes of the right hemisphere (53.4±23.4 and 55.2±6.7, with a norm of 75%).

At the same time, the left temporal lobe plays an important role in auditory-speech memory and information storage, since speech zones are located in this area. At the same time, verbal semantic memory is more associated with the left (dominant) half of the brain, while the right temporal lobe is responsible for episodic memory.

Conclusion

In patients with cerebral infarction in the left and right hemispheres, there are differences in the specifics of disorders of mental activity. When the right hemisphere is affected, the main disorders are inertia of memorization and difficulties in remembering faces and figures. When the left hemisphere is affected, the most pronounced violations are noted in verbal-logical, operative and associative memory.

The analysis of the characteristics of the course of mnestic activity makes it possible to identify syndromic violations of its individual functions. Thus, when the right temporal lobe of the brain is affected, there is a decrease in operational (fixation) memory. The lesion of the right occipital lobe (subdominant) leads to visual memory disorders (memory for faces and geometric shapes). Damage to the occipital and temporal lobes mainly leads to a violation of associative memory. The lesion of the parietal-occipital lobe corresponds to a violation of logical memory. The lesion of the temporal-parietal-occipital lobes of the right hemisphere is characterized by a violation of direct (mechanical memorization) and semantic memory.

Along with the impaired functions, it is important to note the preserved memory components. When the temporal lobe of the right hemisphere is affected, visual and logical memory remain the most functional. In case of damage to the parietal and occipital lobes, the operative (fixation) remains more intact. At the same time, visual and associative memory show high performance in case of parietal lobe deficiency.

References
1. Bizyuk, A.P. (2005). Compendium of neuropsychological research methods. St. Petersburg: Rech.
2. Bleikher, V. M, Kruk, I. V. (1986). Pathopsychological diagnosis. Kyiv.
3. Baddeley, A. (Ed.). (2008). Working memory. Psychology of memory. 3rd ed. M.: Astrel.
4. Grigorieva, V.N., Kovyazina, M.S., Tkhostov, A.Sh. (2006). Cognitive neurorehabilitation of patients with focal brain lesions. M.: Moscow Psychological and Social Institute.
5. Grechenko, T. N. (2010). Neural mechanisms of memory in the works of E. N. Sokolov. Bulletin of Moscow University. [DX Reader version]. Retrieved from http://www.ebiblioteka.ru/sources/article.jsp?issueId=1114045&pager.offset=4.
6. Gudkova, V.V., Shanina, T.V., Stakhovskaya, L.V. (2011). The influence of cognitive disorders on the effectiveness of early rehabilitation of patients who have suffered a stroke. Handbook of a polyclinic doctor. 5. 33-36.
7. Zeigarnik, B.V. (Ed.). (1981). Kurt Lewin’s theory of personality. M.: Moscow State University Publishing House.
8. Korsakova, N.K., Moskovichiute, L.I. (Ed.). (1988). Clinical neuropsychology. M.: MSU.
9. Leontiev, A.N. (Ed.). (1975). Activity, consciousness, personality. M.: Politizdat.
10. Luria, A.R. (Ed.). (1962). Higher cortical functions of humans and their disturbances in local brain lesions. M.: Moscow University Publishing House.
11. Luria, A.R. (Ed.). (1976). Neuropsychology of memory. M.: Pedagogika.
12. Mikadze, Yu.V. (1979). Organization of mnestic activity in patients with local brain lesions (Semantic organization of verbal material and structure of mnestic activity): abstract. dis. Ph.D. psycho. Sciences: 19.00.04. M.
13. Rebrova, N. P., Chernysheva, M. P. (2004). Functional interhemispheric asymmetry of the human brain and mental processes. St. Petersburg: Rech.
14. Skvortsova, V.I., Shetova, I.M., Kakorina, E.P. (2018). Results of the implementation of the Set of measures to improve medical care for patients with acute cerebrovascular accidents in the Russian Federation. Journal of Neurology and Psychiatry named after. C.C. Korsakova. T. 118. 4. 5-12. doi:10.17116/jnevro2018118415-12.
15. Khomskaya, E.D. (Ed.). (2018). Neuropsychology: Textbook for universities. St. Petersburg: Piter.
16. Ebbinghaus, G. (Ed.). (1911). Essay on Psychology. Add., trans. with 3 German ed. edited by [and with a preface] K.I. Povarnina, dir. Pedol. institute, pr.-assoc. Military medical acad. St. Petersburg.
17. Atkinson, R. C., Shiffrin, R. M. (Ed.). (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. New York: Academic Press. Vol. 2. 89-195. doi:10.1017/CBO9781316422250.025
18. Baddeley, A. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of Memory Disorders. England: Wiley.
19. Cicerone, K., Langenbahn, D., Braden, C, et al. (2011). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 2003-2008. Archives Physical Medicine Rehabilitation.
20. Clare, L., Jones, R. (2008). Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impairment. Neuropsychological Review. 18(1), 1–23. doi:10.1007/s11065-008-9051-4
21. Doornhein, K., De Haan, H. F. (1998). Cognitive training for memory deficits in stroke patieDoornhein K., De Haan H. F. Cognitive training for memory deficits in stroke patients. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 8(4). 393 – 400. doi:10.1080/13755579
22. Fish, J., Manly, T., Emslie, H., Evans, J.J., Wilson, B. A. (2008). Compensatory strategies for acquired disorders of memory and planning: Differential effects of a paging system for patients with brain injury of traumatic versus cerebrovascular etiology. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 79(8). 930-935. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2007.125203
23. Hydén, H. (Ed.). (1967). Dynamic aspects on the neuron-glia relationship. A study with micro-chemical methods. The Neuron. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
24. Snaphaan, L., de Leeuw, F.E. (2007). Poststroke memory function in nondemented patients: A systematic review on frequency and neuroimaging correlates. Stroke. 38(1). 198 – 203. doi:10.1116/01.STR.0000251842.34322.8f
25. Turkeltaub, P.E. (2015). Brain Stimulation and the Role of the Right Hemisphere in Aphasia Recovery. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 15(11). 9. doi:10.1007/s11910-015-059

Peer Review

Peer reviewers' evaluations remain confidential and are not disclosed to the public. Only external reviews, authorized for publication by the article's author(s), are made public. Typically, these final reviews are conducted after the manuscript's revision. Adhering to our double-blind review policy, the reviewer's identity is kept confidential.
The list of publisher reviewers can be found here.

The subject of the study in the presented article is neuropsychological syndromes of disorders of mnestic activity in patients with ONMC in the right (subdominant) hemisphere. The descriptive method, the method of categorization, the method of analysis, the method of comparison, and the method of observation were used as the methodology of the subject area of research in this article. In particular, the analysis of the clinical manifestations of right-sided ONMC in patients of the neurological department with a dominant left hemisphere was carried out. The observation was carried out for 14 days, which corresponds to the acute period of ONMC. 3 data measurements were performed: upon admission, after 7 days and upon discharge (14± 1 day). A set of patho- and neuropsychological methods was used to assess memory: the "Wexler Memory Scale", indirect memorization according to A.N. Leontiev, the "Pictograms" technique, Benton's visual retention, the "Memorizing 10 words" technique, the "Memorizing faces" technique, the "Memorizing shapes" technique, the MoSS test. The relevance of the article is beyond doubt, since it is generally believed that ischemic lesion of the dominant hemisphere is a serious disorder that greatly affects the patient's condition and worsens the prognosis for rehabilitation, work capacity and quality of life. It is known that a violation in the basin of the left carotid artery leads to a pronounced cognitive deficit in 46% of cases. At the same time, damage to the vascular basin supplying the subdominant hemisphere causes cognitive deficits in 15% of cases, regardless of the severity of the lesion, that is, patients with both transient attacks and irreversible deeply disabling vascular events are included in the statistics. One way or another, any suffered cerebrovascular accident is a serious case affecting the further fate of the patient. All this points to the enormous medical and social significance of the problem. To date, medical practice has presented data indicating the proven effectiveness of neuropsychological rehabilitation techniques, the most effective is a technique that works according to the mechanism of intersystem functional compensation. Such therapy is a factor in improving prognosis, regardless of the patient's age, gender, initial cognitive status and vascular history. At the same time, the maximum effectiveness of neuropsychological and neurodynamic therapy can be achieved by purposefully training the lost function, in this case, a type of mnestic disorder. Despite the fact that determining the lesion in modern medicine is not difficult due to visualization methods, there is no clear understanding of the attachment of a particular function of higher nervous activity to a specific site. The scientific novelty of the study consists in analyzing the structure of clinical symptoms and features of impaired mnestic function in patients with acute cerebral catastrophe in the right (subdominant) hemisphere using the author's methodology. The article is written in the language of scientific style with the competent use in the text of the study of terms and categories characterizing the subject of the study and the description of the procedure for conducting the study. The structure is designed taking into account the basic requirements for writing scientific articles, such elements as introduction, methods and principles of research, main results, conclusion and bibliography are highlighted in the structure of this study. The content of the article reflects its structure. The study notes that the deterioration of short-term memory is an important symptom of damage to both the dominant and subdominant hemispheres. This symptom is not specific, it has been described in several studies. The peculiarity of this symptom is reversibility. Of course, it is rare to talk about the complete restoration of short-term memory, according to some data, even with intensive cognitive training, it is possible to achieve the initial quality of memory in 40 percent of cases. The diagnosis of this symptom is reduced to a simple test for memorizing 10 words according to the method of A.R. Luria. A satisfactory result is the reproduction of 7 ± 2 words, if it is not known how many points the patient scored before ONMC, if it is known how much the patient memorized before ONMC, then this figure is the minimum goal of rehabilitation. Correction of this symptom is important for the complete rehabilitation of the patient. An extremely effective method of correction is a combination of cognitive gymnastics with physical exercises. The combination of general physical exercises adapted to the patient's condition with game exercises like "repeat after me" or "repeat the movements that I will name" accelerates both spontaneous memory recovery by the mechanism of disinhibition and improves memory due to the formation of cross-functional communications of the central nervous system. Assessment of the affected area and the "dropped out" mnestic function can occur during cognitive testing, as well as in the assessment of symptoms and syndromes. It is the assessment of the symptoms and syndromes (that is, the clinic) of the lesion that makes it possible to plan further cognitive therapy. In this regard, the topic of the work is extremely relevant - a review of the described syndrome in patients with damage to the subdominant hemisphere. The bibliography contains 25 sources, including domestic and foreign periodicals and non-periodicals. The article describes the positions and points of view of scientists characterizing approaches and various aspects of neuropsychological syndromes of disorders of mental activity, and also contains an appeal to scientific works characterizing the subject area of research. The presented study contains conclusions concerning the subject area of the study. In particular, it is noted that patients with cerebral infarction in the left and right hemispheres have differences in the specifics of disorders of mental activity. When the right hemisphere is affected, the main disorders are inertia of memorization and difficulties in remembering faces and figures. When the left hemisphere is affected, the most pronounced violations are noted in verbal-logical, operative and associative memory. The analysis of the characteristics of the course of mnestic activity makes it possible to identify syndromic violations of its individual functions. Thus, when the right temporal lobe of the brain is affected, there is a decrease in operational (fixation) memory. The lesion of the right occipital lobe (subdominant) leads to visual memory disorders (memory for faces and geometric shapes). Damage to the occipital and temporal lobes mainly leads to a violation of associative memory. The lesion of the parietal-occipital lobe corresponds to a violation of logical memory. The lesion of the temporal-parietal-occipital lobes of the right hemisphere is characterized by a violation of direct (mechanical memorization) and semantic memory. Along with the impaired functions, it is important to note the preserved memory components. When the temporal lobe of the right hemisphere is affected, visual and logical memory remain the most functional. In case of damage to the parietal and occipital lobes, the operative (fixation) remains more intact. At the same time, visual and associative memory show high performance in case of parietal lobe deficiency. The materials of this study are designed for a special readership, they can be interesting and used by scientists for scientific purposes, teaching staff in the educational process, psychologists, neuropsychologists, psychotherapists, medical professionals, experts and analysts. As disadvantages of this study, it should be noted that when making tables, it is necessary to pay attention to the requirements of the current GOST. The tables themselves are difficult to perceive, for clarity, it would be advisable to also use figures when describing the main results of the study. In the title of the article, it might be worth avoiding abbreviations (ONMC). These shortcomings do not reduce the high scientific and practical significance of the research itself, but rather relate to the design of the text of the article. It is recommended to publish the article.