Translate this page:
Please select your language to translate the article


You can just close the window to don't translate
Library
Your profile

Back to contents

Philosophy and Culture
Reference:

On the Typology of Musical Perception

Dyatlov Dmitrii Alekseevich

Doctor of Art History

Professor, Piano department, Samara State Institute of Culture

443010, Russia, Samara region, Samara, Frunze str., 167, room 100

diatlovda@mail.ru

DOI:

10.7256/2454-0757.2022.10.38893

EDN:

FMGNPE

Received:

05-10-2022


Published:

12-10-2022


Abstract: Throughout the 20th century, the theory and history of performing arts repeatedly attempted to systematize various phenomena in the field of interpretation of works of academic music. The number of performing styles and types in various researches has risen dramatically, until the beginning of this century when it was reduced to a single concept - to the category of an individual style. This topic was discussed mainly in research papers devoted to the theory of pianism. The results of their observations and conclusions can be applied to the performing arts in general. The article mentions the works of K.A. Martinsen, D.A. Rabinovich, V.P. Chinaeva, A.V. Malinkovskaya and others. The topic of musical perception is addressed both in musicology and in musical psychology. This research offers a new vector for studying of musical perception, linking the features of musical imagery created by the performer and the nature of perception. The following aspects are revelated: patterns that allow us to talk about the types of musical image, the principles of its construction and the types of perception. This is an affective image created on the principle of contrast. It corresponds to the psychophysical type of perception. The contextual image is guided by the principle of connections. It corresponds to the aesthetic type of perception. The integrative image is formed, focusing on the principle of the whole. It corresponds to the symbolic type of perception. A hermetic system of typology is considered to be impossible; artistic practice will largely oppose it. However, these tendencies are able to shed light on the nature of musical perception and allow to continue the scientific discussion of the current topic of musical performance.


Keywords:

musical perception, musical performance, academic music, performing style, affective, aesthetic, symbolic, musical image, type of perception, performing type

This article is automatically translated.

IntroductionAcademic musical art, which originated in the bosom of church music and has centuries-old traditions, is addressed both to man and to the depths of being.

 

As an art of high style, it carries the ethos of service, the pathos of emotional persuasion and the logic of a harmonious musical "construction".  The aspiration of Baroque artists to the Spirit, the Harmony in the "architecture" of musical classicism, the tragic experience of romantic composers of the imperfections of the world - all this remains relevant to this day. The legacy of three centuries is also in demand. This fact is confirmed by the repertoire policy of concert agencies, and confirms the free choice of repertoire by musicians who do not depend on philharmonic management. Both the first and the second focus on the audience. At the same time, their motives are different: the management lives according to the laws of commerce, the musician at the behest of artistic intuition.

The forms of existence of musical art today are extremely diverse. The palette of musical phenomena is variegated and almost immense. Academic music associated with the compositional and, especially, performing tradition of the last two centuries is played in concert halls and attracts the attention of a vast audience. Academic music, the peculiarities of its performance and perception will be discussed in this article.

A composer creating a work, or a musician-performer preparing it for public presentation, as a rule, presuppose a listener's reaction to his work. Even if this is not taken into account, sooner or later the essay becomes public. This is how the well-known triad of composition-performance-perception or composer-performer-listener arises.

Music science, referring to the phenomenon of authorship, has developed categories of style and, more broadly, epoch. The process of periodization of New European music was long and sometimes contradictory. Currently, both the category of style and the boundaries of cultural and historical epochs are recognized as well-established. Interest in the performing arts appeared after such a phenomenon as the "golden age" of the piano, when the keyboard band gradually became a ubiquitous concert practice. Speaking about performing arts and musical perception, we will rely on piano performance, which focuses on various creative intentions – the creator of the music, its performer and, finally, the listener (meaning interested and creatively active perception). Everything that will be said about the phenomenon of piano performance and musical perception can be safely projected onto other types of academic musical art, where the role of personal participation in performance is obvious. Today, piano performance is a wide field of study, it is here that it is possible to identify patterns characteristic of various types and forms of performing art from solo to ensemble.

If the topic of performing types or styles in music science has been developed quite widely, then the problems of musical perception in musicology remain (with some exceptions) on the periphery. The perception of music is occupied by psychology, or rather its local discipline – musical psychology. Like parallel lines in Euclidean geometry, the problems of musicology, interpretology, and the theory of performing arts do not intersect with the problems of musical perception anywhere. Some of their conjugations have a rather generalized character. The purpose of this study is to begin the convergence of the theory of performance and musical psychology, to give rise to a scientific discussion. The correlation of performing styles and features of listener reception, images created by musicians and ways of comprehending them, makes it possible to start a conversation about the typology of musical perception.

 

About the typology of performers or performing styles

 

One of the first researchers of the performing arts was, as is well known, K.A. Martinsen, who proposed a typology of performing musicians. Linking their individual technique with the peculiarities of the sound–making will, he proposed three types - classical, romantic and expressionist [12, p. 92]. Martinsen created his system without taking into account the questions of the sociology of music, without realizing that the phenomena of art do not remain in some kind of immutability, but depend on many factors. This was noted by D.A. Rabinovich in his book "Performer and Style", suggesting in turn a different typology. His "piano" types are changeable and variable, indirectly connected with all movements of the "socio-historical situation" [16, p. 33]. It seems important that Rabinovich represents not only types, but also the goals of their activities: for the virtuoso type – to amaze, emotional – to express, rationalistic – to convince, intellectual - to comprehend. A large-scale study by V.P. Chinaev offers a rather branched "tree" of performing types or, as the author defines, performing styles. Solving an ambitious task "... to substantiate an independent author's concept in which performing styles would be traced historically consistently, contextually broadly, in their versatile ethical, aesthetic and poetic coverage" [20, p. 6], Chinaev builds a multi-level picture of styles. These are classicist and pathetic, brilliant and high romantic, emotional-narrative and intellectual-contextual, narrative-psychological and formal-analytical styles.Non-idiomatic and authentic styles remain somewhat apart from the main path of research. The first relates to avant–garde art, the second to early music. A special place is given to the author's pianism of the first quarter of the XX century. The panorama of musical performance in the second half of the XX – beginning of the XXI century, according to A.V. Malinkovskaya, is determined by the factors of "globalization and acceleration of cultural processes in the world, their "mass character"" [9, p. 37]. In the triad of stylecreative methodindividual-typological status put forward by her, the most important categories are connected, revealing the uniqueness of each noticeable phenomenon in the performing arts and, at the same time, leave the opportunity to determine typical qualities. Speaking about the principles of typology of performers, the author notes that "the most important importance here belongs to the factors of intonation consciousness, intonation thinking of the performer, "centering" and regulating the patterns and mechanisms of interaction of style and method. These are the factors that determine, on the one hand, the uniqueness of each creative personality, on the other – their belonging to certain typological groups" [10, pp. 25-26]. V.N. Kholopova also speaks about the individual performing style and its significance for the content of interpretation [18, p. 298]. We have named only a few works from the whole corpus of performing arts research. They reflect the movement of research thought over several decades (starting with Martinsen's work, published in 1930). It can be noted that throughout the XX century, the typology of performers in various works became more complicated and branched out, until eventually it was reduced to an individual style of performance. Today, new works continue this direction of musicology. Musicology is interested in the finest nuances of individual style [19] and "performing intellectualism" in the performance of the latest compositions [14], problematic issues of the educational discipline "History of performing Styles" [7] and the mechanisms of the phenomenon of individual style traits from the musical imagery of the performed work [5].

 

Musicology and music psychology about listener perception

 

Musical psychology, along with the problems of performance, pays attention to the perception of music, finding significant differences in the "perceptual" capabilities of a particular group of listeners. Speaking about the listener's perception, no matter what qualitative characteristics it is characterized by, we mean aesthetic perception. The multilevel nature of this phenomenon is obvious. The very first reactions of a person to the sound of music are bodily. S.L. Vygotsky in his classic work "Psychology of Art" writes: "Everything that art does, it does in our body and through our body" [3, p. 322]. It is also indisputable that music evokes feelings in the listener, the diversity of which depends on his individual qualities. According to Vygotsky, the affects caused by art are the basis of aesthetic reaction [3, p. 273]. The aesthetic perception itself is a genuine, intense and active creativity of the listener. According to Vygotsky, this is due to the phenomenon of overcoming, he believes that "... it is not enough just to sincerely experience the feeling that the author possessed, it is not enough to understand the structure of the work itself – it is still necessary to creatively overcome your own feeling, find its catharsis, and only then the effect of art will be fully affected" [3, p. 315]. The most important quality of works of high style is their kinship with human speech. The melos of instrumental music always correlates with singing or speech utterance, is built in accordance with the norms of human breathing. This speech expressiveness has an irresistible effect on sensitive perception, whether it is a singing intonation or an instrumental piece, a one-voice line or a polyphonic fabric, a homophonic-harmonic or polyphonic composition of the work. E.V. Nazaikinsky in the book "On the Psychology of musical perception" writes about the relationship of musical and speech intonations as follows: "the principles of constructing replicas ... are largely based on meaningful and genre-colored speech forms, structures of dialogic speech or genre types of monologue" [13, p. 281]. M.S. Bonfeld goes further, speaking about the complex process of understanding music, the perception of which is only a condition for further creative work. He offers his hierarchy of levels of understanding or stages – from the lowest to the highest: stay, contemplation, experience, listening, analysis [1, pp. 215-216]. It should be noted here that at the time of listening to music, it is hardly possible to talk about analytical operations. Listening, however, may indicate a strenuous work of attention, nothing more. The experience speaks of a sensory reaction of perception. But contemplation or staying are obvious signs of an aesthetic reaction. They should be called the highest stage of understanding music, if not limited to its structure, even in a scrupulous competent analysis. If we talk about analysis in the process of perception, then only as an insignificant part of it. Binding processes (synthesis) are more important here than splitting into component parts (analysis). Bonfeld mentions certain "levels of understanding" corresponding to "the nature of penetration into the musical depths" [1, p. 213], coming to the problem of the typology of perception, but does not continue the discussion, remaining with the generalized image of the listener. A.V. Toropova addresses the individual personal qualities of the listener, introducing the categories intonation form of personality, intonation vocabulary of personality, individual tools of sign intonation, integral individuality of intonation. The latter is divided into levels "manifesting personality: the organizational level, the level of the social "I" and the spiritual-personal" [17, p. 269]. Subtle and deep observations of the nature of musical consciousness, musicality as such, reveal important qualitative characteristics of a "musical" personality. But since everyone has their own "instruments of iconic intonation", it is hardly possible to talk about any types of musical consciousness or perception. Like the typology of performing styles, which has been reduced today to the category of individual style, musical psychology reduces all the diversity of artistic reception to the individual qualities of an individual [4],[2].

 

From musical imagery to musical perceptionThe musical horizontal unfolds in the form of melos and recitations, affects perception as a "syntactic" structure, revealing through the means of performing punctuation the discontinuity and coherence of short and extended constructions, communicates with it as a language.

 

It also acts directly, addressing the feeling, causing reciprocal bodily reactions. Being in incessant formation, the sound horizontal is likened to natural processes, thereby captivating the listener's perception, which in turn finds organic naturalness here, "learns" the truth of life in the truth of art. As A.V. Malinkovskaya correctly notes, "melodiousness, horizontal perspective, melosity – all these are ascending stages of reflection in music and performance of the dialectic of life processes, formation" [8, p. 181]. If melodic or recitative constructions can be compared with graphics in fine art, then vertical structures are likened to painting. Harmonies and harmonic complexes, sound texture reveal countless variations of color, astringency and softness, concentration and sparsity, etc. Also an important function is the temporary organization of the sound fabric – uniform or variable meter pulses and diverse rhythm figures. Thus, addressing the listener's feeling, horizontal and vertical constructions, metrical and rhythmic organization of music cause a response resonance in perception – an organic reaction of various affects.

The sound image can carry extra-musical content. Extra-musical connotations arise not in sound as such, but in the field of perception. The listener perceives and recognizes "words" and "phrases" from the "intonation dictionary of the epoch" (B. Asafyev), instantly understands the meanings of "migrating intonation formulas" (L. Shaimukhametova). Here, in addition to the bodily "response" and sensory "resonance", allusions and analogies are included in perception, cultural memory and synesthetic reactions are actively involved. This happens in the perception of program music and non-program compositions as well. Works that do not have a name or even a genre designation carry an impressive amount of extra-musical content. This volume is set by the centuries-old memory of tradition, is associated with organic human life processes, with natural phenomena and elements, with historical or social collisions, with other types of art, etc. Here works the memory of art and the memory of culture, historical memory and the experience of artistic perception of each recipient. In the perception of music in the context of extra-musical content, the conventionality of art becomes most obvious. The depth and completeness of perception depend on the cultural competencies of the audience, its listening experience.

Hearing works as an art of sounds does not imply any other connotations other than musical ones. In this perception, cultural memory is silent, analogies and associations do not come, affects remain on the periphery of consciousness. It does not seek meanings, does not seek understanding. Pure aesthetic contemplation is irrational, does not rely on experience and does not seek generalizations, lives in the present moment, in which the very essence of the musical idea that underlies every work of high style is revealed. V. Kandinsky recalled how, having abandoned the objective world, he learned "not to look at the picture from the outside, but to rotate in the picture himself, to live in it." He recalled how for many years he had been looking for "means to introduce the viewer into the picture so that he would rotate in it, selflessly dissolve in it" [6, pp. 279-280]. For Merab Mamardashvili, the perception of the same nature gives rise to a different effect – not of dissolving into the perceived, but going beyond the boundaries of oneself: "We think with them (objects of art) or in them (and they act ecstatically in us, ecstasize us)" [11, p. 54]. A kind of detachment from making art is experienced by some performing musicians, those of them who tend to trust the depths of intuition and irrational decisions. So Vladimir Sofronitsky said in an interview that he had to "go through so completely to die, and moreover maintain such a state, as if it wasn't me playing, and I had nothing to do with it. There must be some special calmness, and when you get up from the piano, it's as if you weren't playing" [15, p. 77]. As for the listener's perception of such an ecstatic kind, it is not available to everyone. And artistic competence has nothing to do with it, experience and knowledge of the details of art are not important. It requires a kind of giftedness, sensitivity and, most importantly, freedom from preconceived judgments, from right and wrong opinions. This phenomenon of silence, quiet contemplation, and in some cases, standing in front of an unknown, but clearly felt presence, can be based on affects, loaded with extra-musical connotations. Perception of this quality can pass through these stages and immediately turn to the essence of what sounds like a symbolic reality.

The most important quality of New European music is interpretativeness. This quality was one of the reasons for the emergence of the phenomenon of performance. Generations of musicians replace each other, generations of listeners come and go, and interpretations of famous, and sometimes textbook works are multiplying. Each of them actualizes long-composed music, while finding a modern sound. Many of them cause resonance in the audience. The musician consciously or unconsciously strives to meet the request of his listener. In other cases, the listener perceives in the interpretation what he is ready for, or what he can do. Anyway, the palette of ways to hear music is quite wide and multicolored. Is there an opportunity to understand it, to find connections with interpretation, connections with the sounding image? Is it possible to determine the patterns or principles of creating sound imagery corresponding to a particular way of perception?

 

The sounding image, the principle of conformity, the type of perceptionEvery typology operates with vivid qualities and characteristics in order to most accurately determine a particular group of phenomena.

 

Just as there are no pure temperaments in nature, but they are all mixed in one way or another, so in the typology of sounding images and their corresponding types of perception, it is impossible to find "sterile" analogues in real artistic practice. But we cannot do without forced simplifications and some schematization here.

No matter how diverse the concert life is, no matter how bright and "unique" the personalities of the artists are, the images created on the concert stage can still be divided into three large groups. These are affective, contextual and integrative images. They correspond to the principles of contrast, connections and the whole. It is with different kinds of contrasts that the musician operates, creating an affective image. He directly addresses the affects of his listener, causing shock by the extreme amplitude of the loud dynamics from thundering climaxes to the tenderest whisper. The sound perspective in this performance is intentionally deep and extremely transparent. Tempo contrasts are offered, causing a quickening of the heart pulse and deep agitated breathing in the listener. Contextual imagery avoids extremes that can obscure the structure of the sounding, distract from the music, get carried away by one's own feeling. However, it does not cancel sensory perception. But, following the outline of the unfolding intonation plot, it helps to recognize extra-musical content in music, causes allusions, and excites synesthetic reactions. As a result, such imagery is created in which various meanings are connected, it is possible to play with signs and symbols of art, the sounding is immersed in a wide context of meanings. Integrative imagery includes all affective effects, various kinds of analogies and extra-musical correspondences, awakens cultural memory. But at some point it turns out that above all this, going deep into the musical content, it rushes to the very center of the sound meaning to comprehend or accept an artistic idea. In integrative imagery, the principle of the whole operates: at every moment of sounding, we feel that we "hear" the whole work.

As a result, it is possible to identify the types of listener perception that can accommodate and creatively overcome a certain sounding imagery. It is necessary to determine the correspondence of the sounding image, the principle of its creation and the type of perception. So, the affective image created in accordance with the principle of contrast corresponds to the psychophysical type of perception. The contextual image and its principle of connections is an aesthetic type of perception. The integrative image, its principle of the whole, is a symbolic type.

 

ConclusionThe typology of musical perception is only outlined in this article.

 

As already mentioned above, there can be no symbolic, aesthetic, or psychophysical types of perception in its pure form. Once the listener is in the concert hall, he willy-nilly reacts to what is happening. He perceives music with feelings, resonating with what sounds in response; on the basis of experience, cultural memory awakens in him; freedom from bias and artistic sensitivity allow him to penetrate into the mysterious depth of a work of high style. In the proposed typology there are no indications of the stages of perception or its levels – lower, middle, higher. Each of the types corresponds to a special musical ability or even giftedness. They, like temperament, character traits or features of the mental structure are given to a person from birth. Some qualities suggest the possibility of development based on the accumulation of artistic experience. At the same time, feelings will always prevail in an emotional person. Thoughtful is capable of creating intertextual connections. The dreamy one is ready for free contemplation of an artistic subject.

References
1. Bonfeld, M.S. Introduction to Musicology: Textbook for students. higher. studies. institutions / M.S. Bonfeld – M.: Humanit. ed. center VLADOS, 2001. – 224 p.: notes.
2. Boyarintseva, A.A. Perception of music: psychological process, socio-cultural result / A.A. Boyarintseva // Musical culture in theoretical and applied dimension: Collection of scientific articles. – Kemerovo: Kemerovo State Institute of Culture, 2020. – pp. 90-96.
3. Vygotsky L.S. Psychology of Art / General ed. by V.S. Ivanov; Preface by A.N. Leontiev; Commentary by L.S. Vygotsky, V.S. Ivanov. – Moscow: Iskusstvo, 1968. – 576 p.
4. Isaeva, S.A. From music perception to musical perception (Historical and cultural context): abstract of the dissertation for the degree of candidate of cultural studies: 24.00.01 "Theory and history of culture" / Isayeva Svetlana Aleksandrovna – Saransk, 2005. – 18 p.
5. Islyamova, D.R. The performing style of a pianist through the prism of the figurative world of the work / D.R. Islyamova // Problems of modern Science and Education. – 2021. – ¹ 3(160). – Pp. 78-80.
6. Kandinsky V.V. Selected works on the theory of art: Volume 1.1901-1914. / V.V. Kandinsky – M.: Gilea, 2001. – 390 p.
7. Kurgan, O.A. The significance of the discipline "history of performing styles" in the training of musicians, teachers and performers / O.A. Kurgan // The creative heritage of the composers of the Vienna School: history, pedagogy, interpretation: A collection of materials of the IV International Scientific and Practical Conference, Belgorod, May 15, 2021. – Belgorod: Belgorod State Institute of Arts and Culture, 2021. – pp. 56-61.
8. Malinkovskaya, A.V. Piano-performing intonation: Problems of artistic intonation on the piano and analysis of their development in the methodological and theoretical literature of the XVI-XX centuries: Essays. / A.V. Malinkovskaya – M.: Music, 1990. – 186c.
9. Malinkovskaya, A.V. Interpretive and classification aspects of the study of performing styles in the works of Russian researchers of piano art / A.V. Malinkovskaya // Bulletin of the UNESCO Chair of Musical Art and Education. – 2017. – ¹ 3(19). – Pp. 27-41.
10. Malinkovskaya, A.V. Categorical triad "style – creative method – individual typological status" as a methodological basis for studying the art of a performing musician /A.V. Malinkovskaya // Bulletin of the UNESCO Chair "Musical Art and Education" – 2017. – No. 1 (17). – S. 13-32.
11. Mamardashvili, M.K. The Arrow of knowledge. A sketch of natural history epistemology. School "Languages of Russian culture" / M.K. Mamardashvili. – M.: Progress, 1996. – 303 p.
12. Martinsen Karl Adolf. Individual piano technique based on sound-making will / Karl Adolf Martinsen – M.: Music, 1966. – 220 p.
13. Nazaikinsky E.V. On the psychology of musical perception / E.V. Nazaikinsky. – M.: Music, 1972. – 384 p.
14. Nekrasov, N.N. Performing intellectualism in the context of the interpretation of music of the second half of the XX century /N.N. Nekrasov // Music and Time. – 2020. – No. 9. – PP. 33-37.
15. Pianists tell: Collection: Issue 1 / Comp., total. ed. and intro. article M.G. Sokolov. – M.: Music, 1990. – 173 p.
16. Rabinovich, D.A. Performer and style / D.A. Rabinovich – M.: Classics-XXI, 2008. – 208 p. – (Master class).
17. Toropova, A.V. The intonating nature of the psyche: musical and psychological anthropology / A.V. Toropova; Moscow Pedagogical State University – Moscow: Moscow Pedagogical State University, 2018. – 338 p.
18. Kholopova, V.N. Music as a form of art: a textbook / V.N. Kholopova. – 4th ed., Rev. – St. Petersburg: Planet of Music, 2014. – 320 p.
19. Huang, Sh. On the typology of the individual style of the musician-performer / Sh. Huang // Bulletin of the Belarusian State Pedagogical University. Series 1. Pedagogy. Psychology. Philology. – 2020. – ¹ 4(106). – Pp. 53-56.
20. Chinaev, V.P. Performing styles in the context of artistic culture of the XVIII-XX centuries (On the example of piano performing art): abstract of the dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Art History: 17.00.02 "Musical art" / Chinaev, Vladimir Petrovich – Moscow: 1995. – 48 p.

Peer Review

Peer reviewers' evaluations remain confidential and are not disclosed to the public. Only external reviews, authorized for publication by the article's author(s), are made public. Typically, these final reviews are conducted after the manuscript's revision. Adhering to our double-blind review policy, the reviewer's identity is kept confidential.
The list of publisher reviewers can be found here.

The subject of the study, musical perception, is considered in the context of the figurative content of musical works. The author substantiates and expounds his own way of typologizing musical perception based on the explication of the accumulated musicological experience of the typology of musical performance and the characteristics of musical and stylistic epochs in the designated subject area (musical perception). Revealing the experience of the typology of musical works and their interpretations, the author justifiably points out the possibility of a typology of musical perception on the same principle and reveals the subject from a new perspective: not from the psychology of perception, but from the content of musical works. The basis for the author's typology is the ways of organizing the musical image ("affective, contextual and integrative"). Rejecting the typology of perception by levels, the author fixed three ways of perception: the affective image corresponds to the psychophysical type of perception, the contextual image corresponds to the aesthetic type of perception, the integrative image corresponds to the symbolic type. The author's observation of the general principle of creation (composer), interpretation (performer) and perception (listener) of a musical image (or rather, the imaginative sphere of a particular musical work) is essential. Three heterogeneous principles correspond to the three types of construction of musical images and their perception: contrast, connection (or context) and integrity (symbolism). The discovered relationship is the strength of the author's reasoning. The author does not rank the listener's ability to perceive music in one way or another and claims that this ability remains innate. Of course, the author may have his own position on this issue, but it should also be pointed out that there is a logical contradiction in it: the higher mental abilities of an individual are acquired and socially historically conditioned (L.S. Vygotsky). The reliance on Vygotsky's socio-historical theory of the psychology of art contradicts the author's statement about being gifted with the ability to a particular type of musical perception from birth. Although this fundamental contradiction does not cast doubt on the result achieved by the author (the author's typology of musical perception). One can also refer to an empirical argument that refutes the author's thesis about the innateness of the type of musical perception. The sonata form of musical works has developed historically. In Beethoven's work, it includes a complex dramaturgy of images, combining all three principles of building a musical image given by the author. Accordingly, having only one specific innate type of perception, both the listener and the performer are unable to read Beethoven. What kind of innate type of musical perception do performers/listeners who consider themselves to be Beethoven fans have? An unaccounted-for fourth? According to the reviewer, the author should have explained such a paradoxical conclusion. Perhaps we are talking exclusively about innate predispositions to prefer one or another type of musical perception. At the same time, the author does not deny that "some qualities [of musical perception] suggest the possibility of development based on the accumulation of artistic experience," which still allows us to recommend the article for publication. The research methodology is based on the general theoretical method of typology, enhanced comparison, generalization and explication of theoretical models from a well-developed field of musicology into a little-studied subject area. The logic of the author's reasoning reveals new horizons for observing musical perception using the author's typology. The proposed methodological innovation has, in the opinion of the reviewer, serious heuristic potential and is extremely valuable in itself. The demarcation of the types of musical perception indicated by the author reveals the prospects of applying a new typology to improve the art of musical performance. The relevance of the presented research is due to the existing gap in the study of musical perception from a musicological perspective based on the patterns (principles) of the organization of the musical image. The scientific novelty is quite obvious and well-founded. The author's typology of musical perception is applicable in further research both in terms of improving performance skills and in musical education and educational practices. The style of the work is scientific. The structure of the article reveals the logic of presenting the results of scientific research. There are no comments on the content of the text. The bibliography fully reveals the problem area, is designed in accordance with the requirements of the editorial board. The appeal to the opponents is correct, sufficient and appropriate. The article is certainly of interest to the readership of the journal "Philosophy and Culture".